The study in
PLO
|
Credit: deviantart |
S Biology therefore suggests that there is a single region of the
brain where both positive and negative feelings for faces take shape and
provides the second demonstration this year that the MRI technique can
be used to train a mental process in an unknowing subject. This spring,
the team used the same method to associate the perception of color with
the context of a pattern so strongly that volunteers saw the color when
cued by the pattern, even if the color wasn't really there.
In the new study, the researchers sought to determine whether they could
direct feelings about faces -- a more sophisticated brain function that
is closer to their eventual goal, which is to develop the technique to
the point where it could become a tool for psychological therapy, for
instance for anxiety.
"Face recognition is a very important social function for people," said
co-author Takeo Watanabe, the Fred M. Seed Professor of Cognitive and
Linguistic Sciences at Brown University. "Facial recognition is
associated with people's emotions."
Decoded neurofeedback explained
The technique, which the researchers call "DecNef," for decoded
neurofeedback, starts with detecting and analyzing the specific activity
patterns in a brain region that correspond to a mental state. For
example, at the beginning of the new study, while 24 volunteers saw
hundreds of faces and rated their sentiments about each of them (on a
scale of 1 for dislike to 10 for like, with 5 for neutral), the
researchers used MRI to record the patterns of activity in a brain
region called the cingulate cortex.
That step alone was fairly conventional neuroscience except that many
scientists believe that positive or negative feelings about faces are
formulated in separate brain regions. But this team of four researchers
at Brown University and the Advanced Telecommunications Research
Institute International in Kyoto, Japan, wanted to test whether the
cingulate cortex handles both sides of the emotion.
Sure enough, the researchers' software, called a decoder, was able to
analyze the recordings to identify reliable and distinct patterns in
each volunteer's cingulate cortex associated with positive and negative
feelings about faces.
"We found that the cingulate cortex seems to handle both opposing
directions with different activity patterns," said co-author Yuka
Sasaki, associate professor (research) of cognitive, linguistic and
psychological sciences at Brown.
With these signature patterns established for each volunteer, the
participants were then unknowingly divided into two groups of 12 --
either positive or negative -- and were called back in for a few days of
additional research in the MRI machine. In this phase the subjects were
shown a subset of the faces they rated as neutral and were then asked
to perform a seemingly unrelated task: After seeing each face on the
screen, they were then shown a disk and asked to somehow use their minds
to try to make it appear as big as possible. The bigger they could make
the disk, they were told, the more of a small monetary reward they
could receive.
In reality, the tasks weren't unrelated. Participants didn't know this
at the time, but the only way the disk would grow was when the MRI
readings showed that they happened (for whatever reason) to produce
their signature patterns of positive or negative feelings about faces in
their cingulate cortex. In other words, the experiment rewarded
volunteers in the positive group with a larger disk when they produced
the pattern associated with liking the faces after seeing a previously
neutral one. Similarly, the experiment rewarded volunteers in the
negative group with a growing disk the more they happened to produce the
pattern associated with dislike after seeing a neutral one.
In essence, DecNef aims to train people to produce specific feelings or
perceptions in specific contexts by rewarding those moments when they
unknowingly do so.
A third group of six other participants was used as a control group.
They saw faces and rated them, but were not given the DecNef step of
having to enlarge a disk in association with the activation patterns
that represent positive or negative feelings.
Finally, all the participants were then queried anew about their feelings regarding the initially neutral faces.
Facial feelings were affected
When the researchers analyzed the results, they were able to make
several key findings. On average, the positive group's ratings of the
neutral faces moved up mildly but significantly (by about 0.6 on the 1
to 10 scale), while the negative group's ratings of the faces moved down
a bit less but still significantly. Meanwhile the control group's
ratings didn't change significantly at all.
"From all these results we conclude that association of originally
neutrally rated faces with covert induction of activity patterns in the
single brain region, the cingulate cortex, led to changes in facial
preference specifically for those faces, and in a specific preference --
positive or negative -- direction," the authors wrote in the study.
To be as certain as possible about the findings, they did a few more
analyses. In post-experiment interviews, they asked the subjects whether
they knew what was really going on -- none did. Then the researchers
explained what the experiment was really about and asked people to say
whether they thought they were in the positive or negative group. People
were no better than chance at saying which they were in. Together these
results suggest that none of the experimental volunteers changed their
preferences about neutral faces based on their own will or intention.
In another analysis, the researchers crunched the numbers to see if the
degree of activity in the cingulate cortex during the disk-enlargement
phase correlated with the degree of change in preferences. The results
revealed a high correlation (0.78 out of 1). In other words, the amount
of brain activity was proportional to the amount of induced feeling.
Toward a DecNef therapy
While the induced changes in feeling were mild, the training took place
over only a few days, the researchers noted. Training that occurs on the
scale of weeks, as is often required for clinical therapies, might have
induced stronger feelings.
But even a small effect could be beneficial for people if it blunts a
persistently painful feeling associated with a certain trigger, Watanabe
said.
"If someone develops a traumatic memory that makes him or her suffer,
even a small reduction of the suffering would be helpful," Watanabe
said.
The researchers also said they are aware that there could be potential
abuse or misuse of the technique -- a kind of brainwashing -- so it
might be good if it proves at least somewhat limited in its effect.
In addition to Sasaki and Watanabe, the paper's other authors are lead
author Kazuhisa Shibata and corresponding author Mitsuo Kawato.